Study reveals why we trust some strangers and not others

Researchers probe how we make an initial judgment on whether to trust or cooperate with others

From getting into a taxi to asking a fellow train passenger to keep an eye on your luggage while buying a coffee, we’ve all put our trust in those we do not know.

Now researchers have revealed that strangers are more likely to be trusted if they look like someone who has earned your trust before – and more likely to be distrusted if they resemble someone who has betrayed your faith in them.

While previous research has shown how people can learn whether others are trustworthy over time, the team say it was unclear how an initial judgment is made about whether to trust or cooperate with someone.

“What we wanted to figure out was what happens when you come across somebody for the first time,” said Dr Oriel FeldmanHall, co-author of the research and a social neuroscientist from Brown University.

Writing in the journal PNAS, a team of researchers in the US reveal how they asked 29 participants to either keep $10 or invest all – or part – of it with one of three men they did not know but whose photographs they were shown.

Over the course of 45 games, 15 for each photograph, participants discovered that while one of the three men frequently shared the profit from the investment, another only shared the money 60% of the time, while the third very rarely shared it at all.

The team then carried out a second experiment in which participants were asked to pick a partner for a new game: either a player whose face they couldn’t see, or a player whose face they were shown in a photograph. While four were completely new faces, 54 photographs were images that had been tinkered with so the picture looked more or less like players from the previous game. Each photo was shown twice, and FeldmanHall says the team were careful to make sure they differed enough to be unrecognisable as the faces used in the first task.

The results reveal that the more a possible player looked like the trusted individual from the previous game, the more likely participants were to select them as their partner for the next task, while an even stronger negative effect was found for those who resembled the untrustworthy man in the initial game. Just over 68% of participants turned down the pictured player if he bore any resemblance to the untrustworthy man.

FeldmanHall noted the findings are similar to the seminal experiments in which Russian scientist Ivan Petrovich Pavlov’s dogs learned to associate the sound of a certain bell with food. “If Pavlov would ring a similar type of bell, the dog would also salivate – it would just salivate a little bit less,” said FeldmanHall.

The team then carried out the same experiments with 28 new participants, while fMRI brain scanning took place.

Among their findings, the team discovered that as the image of the potential candidate was tweaked to look more like the untrustworthy player of the initial game, activity in the amygdala – an area of the brain linked to processing threat – became stronger.

Antonio Espín, a behavioural economist from Middlesex University, London, said the study’s implications could be wide-ranging. “Interestingly, since the main reason for facial similarity is shared genes, the study not only advances our understanding of why we trust or distrust specific strangers but also has broader implications, for example for ethnic or racial discrimination and in the evolutionary arena of partner selection.”

Contributor

Nicola Davis

The GuardianTramp

Related Content

Article image
Empathic people quicker to learn to help others, study shows
Although people are slower to learn to do tasks that help other people, research participants rated as more empathic proved to be quicker learners

Nicola Davis

15, Aug, 2016 @7:00 PM

Article image
Magic mushrooms 'reboot' brain in depressed people – study
Patients unresponsive to conventional treatments benefit when treated with natural psychoactive compound, but researchers warn against self medication

Haroon Siddique

13, Oct, 2017 @9:00 AM

Article image
People with more friends have higher pain thresholds, study suggests
Link between larger friendship circles and high pain thresholds may be down to the way the the brain’s endorphin system has evolved, researchers say

Nicola Davis

28, Apr, 2016 @9:00 AM

Article image
Shocking discovery? Money earned by exploitation is less rewarding, study shows
Experiment involving theoretical financial reward in exchange for uncomfortable electric shocks on self or others reveals brain preference for less pain and lower profit

Hannah Devlin Science correspondent

01, May, 2017 @3:00 PM

Article image
Love hormone helps autistic children bond with others, study shows

Researchers find that nasal sprays laced with oxytocin help youngsters interact better, but the effects do not last long

Ian Sample, science correspondent

02, Dec, 2013 @8:00 PM

Article image
Apes can guess what others are thinking - just like humans, study finds
Research indicates apes are able to predict one another’s beliefs and suggests that other primates have complex inner lives

Hannah Devlin

06, Oct, 2016 @6:00 PM

Article image
AI will create 'useless class' of human, predicts bestselling historian
Smarter artificial intelligence is one of 21st century’s most dire threats, writes Yuval Noah Harari in follow-up to Sapiens

Ian Sample Science editor

20, May, 2016 @12:20 PM

Article image
Breakthrough in understanding the chills and thrills of musical rapture
How certain pieces of music send tingles up the spine has stumped researchers for centuries, but a recent brain scan study may have provided some clues

Ian Sample Science editor

17, Jun, 2016 @1:34 PM

Article image
Creative thought has a pattern of its own, brain activity scans reveal
People who are flexible, original thinkers show signature forms of connectivity in their brains, study shows

Ian Sample Science editor

15, Jan, 2018 @8:00 PM

Article image
Brain training could help the heartbroken, says neuropsychologist
Computerised tests could train those suffering unrequited love to avoid actions they might later regret, says Cambridge University professor Barbara Sahakian

Ian Sample Science editor

08, Jun, 2017 @3:01 PM