Sweet talk: wild birds and human honey hunters converse, study shows

Honeyguide birds are well known for signalling human honey hunters, but research shows that the communication goes both ways

Wild birds and humans can engage in two-way conversation to the benefit of both, researchers have revealed.

A brown bird with a penchant for beeswax, the greater honeyguide is well known for signalling to human honey hunters, issuing a squeaky, chirruping call while darting between trees to indicate the way to a bees’ nest. Once there the honey-hunters collect honey while the birds feast upon leftover wax.

But now researchers have found the communication goes both ways.

“Honey hunters use special calls to signal to honeyguides that they are eager to follow, and then honeyguides in turn use that information to chose partners who are likely to be good collaborators,” said Claire Spottiswoode, lead author of the research from the University of Cambridge and the University of Cape Town.

While such displays of biological teamwork are common between animals that have been domesticated or trained, cases involving free-living animals are rare. “It seems to be a two-way conversation between our own species and a wild animal from which both those partners benefit,” Spottiswoode added.

Writing in the journal Science, Spottiswoode and colleagues describe how they spent time in the Niassa National Reserve in northern Mozambique with honey hunters of the Yao community to unpick the nature of their relationship with the honeyguide.

The researchers found that when the honey hunters special call was used, around 75% of the time the honeyguide successfully led the way to a bees’ nest.

But the question remained whether the honey hunters’ call specifically signalled to the birds that they wanted to be guided, or whether the birds simply turned up because they recognised that humans were in the vicinity.

“This was instantly intriguing: could this really be an example of reciprocal communication between humans and a wild animal?” said Spottiswoode.

To find out, the researchers took part in 72 honey-hunting expeditions. During each of them one of three different sounds was played, one of which was the honey-hunters’ halloo: a long trill followed by a short grunt with a rising inflection known as the “brrrr-hm” call. The other two sounds, spoken words in the local language and the sounds of a common bird, were used as “controls” for comparison.

The researchers found that the honeyguides were more than twice as likely to lead the way when the “brrrr-hm” call was played compared to the other two sounds, prompting the bird to guide the party on 16 out of the 24 occasions it was played.

“[The call] reliably tells [the bird] that this just isn’t any human, this is a human who wants to cooperate,” said Spottiswoode.

What’s more, the researchers found that the “brrr-hm” call was more than three times as likely to result in the hunters actually finding a bees’ nest than the other sounds, with 54% of ventures proving successful. “We found that when we made the control sounds the honeyguides sometimes lost interest and stopped guiding,” said Spottiswoode.

While the Yao honey hunters have a particular call for the honeyguides, other cultural groups use different sounds to signal to the birds. That, the authors suggest, could mean that the honeyguides develop their own culture reflecting that of the humans they interact with.

Spottiswoode believes the new research points to an intriguing conclusion. “This suggests that honeyguides seem to attach meaning, and respond appropriately, to the signal that specifically advertises people’s’ willingness to cooperate,” she said.

Stuart West, professor of evolutionary biology at the University of Oxford agrees. “The honeyguide kind of understands what the human is saying,” says West. “If we think of domesticated animals, pets and livestock, we can train them to respond to specific signals,” he added, “but to have something like that in a wild animal is quite special.”

Contributor

Nicola Davis

The GuardianTramp

Related Content

Article image
Birds in love produce more babies, study shows | @GrrlScientist
GrrlScientist: A new study finds that birds who freely choose their own mates have 37 percent more offspring than those which were paired up by researchers in a sort of avian ‘arranged marriage’ — findings that have far-reaching implications for conservation and captive breeding practices

GrrlScientist

14, Sep, 2015 @6:00 PM

Article image
Birds identify good nuts by listening to them | @GrrlScientist
GrrlScientist: Wild birds identify “good” seeds without first opening the shells by weighing them and by listening to the sound produced when clicking their beaks on the shell, according to a recent study

GrrlScientist

26, May, 2015 @5:01 PM

Article image
The cold that binds: tiny birds snuggle for warmth | @GrrlScientist
GrrlScientist: A large group of tiny birds huddle together for warmth in a garage near Dunedin New Zealand, as a big snowstorm approaches

GrrlScientist

02, Jun, 2015 @7:15 AM

Article image
Birds pick nest materials with camouflage in mind | @GrrlScientist
GrrlScientist: A recent study by a research team in Scotland reveals that birds intentionally choose colour-matching materials to camouflage their nests thereby reducing predation risk.

GrrlScientist

14, Jan, 2015 @2:14 PM

Article image
Twitter storm: noise pollution creates havoc for birds, study shows
Human activities could be affecting reproduction and even normal social behaviour

Fiona Harvey Environment correspondent

19, Jun, 2019 @11:01 PM

Article image
'Falcon cam' reveals how the birds of prey close in for the kill

Tiny video cameras provide a striking, falcon's-eye view as the birds use 'motion camouflage' to fool hapless crows

Ian Sample, science correspondent

16, Jan, 2014 @7:00 AM

Article image
Froggy style: unique mating position of Bombay night frogs finally revealed
The frogs are secretive, breed only at night, and always at the peak of monsoon season, but researchers have finally identified their remarkable mating habits

Ian Sample Science editor

14, Jun, 2016 @10:58 AM

Article image
Goffin’s cockatoos able to use toolset to complete tasks
Parrot can figure out how to use a tool, pick the most suitable one and even transport a set together

Nicola Davis Science correspondent

10, Feb, 2023 @4:00 PM

Article image
Social rules help varied personalities work as a team, fish study shows
Stickleback foraged more efficiently with conventions present than when individuals behaved independently

Nicola Davis, Science correspondent

02, Mar, 2023 @7:00 PM

Article image
Swifts spend ten months a year entirely airborne, study reveals
Research using miniature tracking devices suggests that swifts eat and sleep in the sky, as some birds did not land at all during their migratory period

Hannah Devlin Science correspondent

27, Oct, 2016 @4:00 PM